This page was originally published in 2007, was later amended, and continues to state chief principles in a useful way. Some later developments are listed ( ... ) below. All the pages on the site are listed in the ( ... ) Quad Net Site Map.
"Brains differ in some rather obvious ways from present-day computers: memory and computation are not separated as they are in all of our current machines; the nervous system operates without any systemwide clock and is built from stochastic elements. Finally, developing as well as mature brains are constantly reprogramming themselves, up or down regulating synaptic weights, modulating the degree of adaptation, shifting the character and frequency of central pattern generators, changing the time constants of integration, and so on. Conceptually, this amounts to the input changing the transition function governing how the machine switches from one state to the next. Put differently, a nervous system will act like a machine that changes its instruction set as a function of its input."Koch's statement is a confusing collection of temporal adjustments that have no inherent basis in a computer model. I do not understand "without any systemwide clock," since I possess at least one in my brain that controls my daily activities like sleep. ["Sleep" is incongruous with a computer model; the "eigen-phase" in Quad Net models (Quad Nets, § 2.c) suggests a reason for sleep, namely, so that neurons can "synchronize their clocks.") In contrast to computers' inability to deal directly with time, levels of temporal adjustment in the Quad Nets model are organized by the inherent design. On the longest time scale, there are adjustments of junctions that correspond to "regulating synaptic weights." Operations can incorporate an inbuilt source of "circadian" or daily rhythms; as well as other rhythmic sources, like sources of musical beats and muscular movements. Inside the daily time scale is a "situational time scale," corresponding, e.g., to movements of a student from home to school to homeroom to math class to physical education to lunch to English class to band practice, etc. Each situation calls for a different distribution of blood flow (energy flow) into the various brain parts. A particular task in a situation calls for fixed signals that will change when the task changes. A shorter time scale involves variations in timing intervals that govern activities of a brain part, like the variations in β and/or δ that control the activities of the 4-cycle, described above. Finally, at the shortest time scale, are the intervals between pulses, e.g., τ=4δ in the 4-cycle.
"Although the areas of the brain that respond to individual pitches have been mapped, we have not yet been able to find the neurological basis for the encoding of pitch relations...These relations must be extracted by computational processes that remain poorly understood."Timing device processes can generate pitch relations, but the processes are not computational. This principle is discussed in An Ear for Pythagorean Harmonics: Brain Models Built From Timing Devices (12/28/2009), a web page with a link to a full technical presentation (.pdf file - 460 kB). These advanced developments have grown out of the materials on this website for which there is an earlier version, titled An Ear for Pythagorean Harmonics: Mathematical Processing in Brain Models Built From "Timing Devices," a .pdf file, 212 kB. The image below shows an early design of an Ear for Pythagorean harmonics. When the frequencies of the two input pulse trains are in a specified relation, e.g., the "dominant" relation, a signal appears on the appropriate output. The signal is the result of a "silence" on the appropriate line coming out of the "frequency fractionator." Silences in timing device designs, like engineers' "nulls" and the mathematical "0," are guides and standards of reference. E.g., when the "following" system above is "moving straight ahead directly toward the source of sensation," there are operational silences at pivotal points that are lost when the balance is tilted.
"We know that there are neural circuits specifically related to detecting and tracking musical meter, and we know that the cerebellum is involved in setting an internal clock or timer that can synchronize with events that are out-there-in-the-world."Again, at p.174:
"The cerebellum is the part of the brain that is involved closely with timing and coordinated movements of the body. ... The function of this oldest part of the brain is something that is crucial to music: timing."However, according to Levitin (p. 172):
"The only way we can feel or know these timing variations is if a computational system in the brain is extracting information about when the beats are supposed to occur."
( ... ) Opening Page
( ... ) A Kit of Parts ( ... ) An Eye for Sharp Contrast ( ... ) Eyes That Look at Objects ( ... ) An Ear for Pythagorean Harmonics ( ... ) A Procrustean Group of Harmonies ( ... ) Fundamentals of Timing Devices
( ... ) top of this page |
( ... ) top of page.
Your comments and suggestions are welcome. Please write to the adjacent email address (shown in an image to minimize spam). |